TOKYO: Stronger brain activity after writing on paper than smartphone, finds study

TOKYO: Stronger brain activity after writing on paper than smartphone, finds study

TOKYO: Writing on a physical paper can
lead to more brain activity when remembering the information an hour later,
suggest the findings of a recent study led by Japanese university students and
recent graduates.

Researchers say that the unique, complex, spatial and tactile information
associated with writing by hand on physical paper is likely what leads to
improved memory.

“Actually, the paper is more advanced and useful compared to electronic
documents because paper contains more one-of-a-kind information for stronger
memory recall,” said Professor Kuniyoshi L. Sakai, a neuroscientist at the
University of Tokyo and corresponding author of the research recently published
in Frontiers in Behavioral Neuroscience. The research was completed with
collaborators from the NTT Data Institute of Management Consulting.

Contrary to the popular belief that digital tools increase efficiency,
volunteers who used paper completed the note-taking task about 25 per cent
faster than those who used digital tablets or smartphones.

Although volunteers wrote by hand both with pen and paper or stylus and digital
tablet, researchers say paper notebooks contain more complex spatial
information than digital paper. Physical paper allows for tangible permanence,
irregular strokes, and uneven shape, like folded corners. In contrast, the
digital paper is uniform, has no fixed position when scrolling, and disappears
when you close the app.

“Our take-home message is to use paper notebooks for information we need
to learn or memorize,” said Sakai.

In the study, a total of 48 volunteers read a fictional conversation between
characters discussing their plans for two months in the near future, including
14 different class times, assignment due dates and personal appointments.
Researchers performed pre-test analyses to ensure that the volunteers, all
18-29 years old and recruited from university campuses or NTT offices, were
equally sorted into three groups based on memory skills, personal preference
for digital or analog methods, gender, age and other aspects.

Volunteers then recorded the fictional schedule using a paper datebook and pen,
a calendar app on a digital tablet and a stylus, or a calendar app on a large
smartphone and a touch-screen keyboard. There was no time limit and volunteers
were asked to record the fictional events in the same way as they would for
their real-life schedules, without spending extra time to memorize the
schedule.

After one hour, including a break and an interference task to distract them
from thinking about the calendar, volunteers answered a range of simple (When
is the assignment due?) and complex (Which is the earlier due date for the
assignments?) multiple choice questions to test their memory of the schedule.
While they completed the test, volunteers were inside a magnetic resonance
imaging (MRI) scanner, which measures blood flow around the brain. This is a
technique called functional MRI (fMRI), and increased blood flow observed in a
specific region of the brain is a sign of increased neuronal activity in that
area.

Participants who used a paper datebook filled in the calendar within about 11
minutes. Tablet users took 14 minutes and smartphone users took about 16
minutes. Volunteers who used analog methods in their personal life were just as
slow at using the devices as volunteers who regularly use digital tools, so
researchers are confident that the difference in speed was related to
memorization or associated encoding in the brain, not just differences in the
habitual use of the tools.
Volunteers who used analog methods scored better than other volunteers only on
simple test questions. However, researchers say that the brain activation data
revealed significant differences.

Volunteers who used paper had more brain activity in areas associated with
language, imaginary visualization, and in the hippocampus — an area known to
be important for memory and navigation. Researchers say that the activation of
the hippocampus indicates that analog methods contain richer spatial details
that can be recalled and navigated in the mind’s eye.

“Digital tools have uniform scrolling up and down and standardized
arrangement of text and picture size, like on a webpage. But if you remember a
physical textbook printed on paper, you can close your eyes and visualize the
photo one-third of the way down on the left-side page, as well as the notes you
added in the bottom margin,” Sakai explained.

Researchers say that personalizing digital documents by highlighting,
underlining, circling, drawing arrows, handwriting colour-coded notes in the
margins, adding virtual sticky notes, or other types of unique mark-ups can
mimic analog-style spatial enrichment that may enhance memory.

Although they have no data from younger volunteers, researchers suspect that
the difference in brain activation between analog and digital methods is likely
to be stronger in younger people.

“High school students’ brains are still developing and are so much more
sensitive than adult brains,” said Sakai.
Although the current research focused on learning and memorization, the
researchers encourage using paper for creative pursuits as well.

“It is reasonable that one’s creativity will likely become more fruitful
if prior knowledge is stored with stronger learning and more precisely
retrieved from memory. For art, composing music, or other creative works, I
would emphasize the use of paper instead of digital methods,” said Sakai.

Leave a Comment